Skip to main content

Environmental and genetic influences on early attachment

Abstract

Attachment theory predicts and subsequent empirical research has amply demonstrated that individual variations in patterns of early attachment behaviour are primarily influenced by differences in sensitive responsiveness of caregivers. However, meta-analyses have shown that parenting behaviour accounts for about one third of the variance in attachment security or disorganisation. The exclusively environmental explanation has been challenged by results demonstrating some, albeit inconclusive, evidence of the effect of infant temperament. In this paper, after reviewing briefly the well-demonstrated familial and wider environmental influences, the evidence is reviewed for genetic and gene-environment interaction effects on developing early attachment relationships. Studies investigating the interaction of genes of monoamine neurotransmission with parenting environment in the course of early relationship development suggest that children's differential susceptibility to the rearing environment depends partly on genetic differences. In addition to the overview of environmental and genetic contributions to infant attachment, and especially to disorganised attachment relevant to mental health issues, the few existing studies of gene-attachment interaction effects on development of childhood behavioural problems are also reviewed. A short account of the most important methodological problems to be overcome in molecular genetic studies of psychological and psychiatric phenotypes is also given. Finally, animal research focusing on brain-structural aspects related to early care and the new, conceptually important direction of studying environmental programming of early development through epigenetic modification of gene functioning is examined in brief.

Early attachment: evolutionary basis and individual variability

Early attachment, a specific personal relationship developing between an infant and the caregiver has been considered essential for survival as well as for later physical and mental development in primates including the human species [1]. The human newborn, however competent in many ways [2], cannot survive unless responsive adults feed and protect them from environmental hazards. Beyond physical care, early experiences have a significant formative influence on children's later mental health, social adjustment and personality development. Attachment theory, conceived by John Bowlby in an evolutionary framework, has proposed that the human infant is born with a set of behavioural mechanisms selected for increasing the chances of survival. The dyadic regulatory system and associated behaviours are normally activated by impending or perceived external danger and by states of internal stress, such as illness or fatigue. Thus, unlike earlier theories of parent-child relationships, which emphasized the role of (any) caregiver in satisfying the infant's physiological needs (e.g., hunger), attachment theory focuses on the selectivity of personal relationships providing protection and emotional security. The attachment behavioural system is theorised to be integrated with other behavioural systems in reaching its "set goal", the felt security of the child under various external and internal conditions. The actual behaviours of the dyadic attachment system clearly change in the course of physical and mental development of children, who come to be able to endure longer separations and increasing distances from caregivers.

Attachment relationships are formed in the course of interactions with caregivers. Infants accumulate information regarding readiness, quality and reliability of responses from others and, by the end of the first year of life, specific representations are formed about the caregivers, the self and the nature of relationships. These representations are theorised to influence children's behaviour contemporaneously, as well as influencing their subsequent social relationships [3].

Variations of attachment patterns

Individual variations in infants' attachment to caregivers can be observed through their behaviour, especially under conditions of stress that activate seeking and maintaining proximity. The Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) [4] developed for measuring the balance between infants' exploration and attachment behaviour consists of short episodes inducing mild stress in the infant by the entrance and approach of a stranger and two subsequent brief separations from the caregiver. Dyadic behaviour videotaped throughout the session is evaluated by experts for fundamental attachment strategies [5] and for the degree of disorganization of these strategies [6]. Secure (B) attachment can be characterized by the infants' open communication of emotions and their ability to make use of the caregiver as a secure base from which to explore. There are two organized insecure attachment patterns; infants avoiding contact or interaction with the caregiver upon reunion and minimizing the expression of negative emotions under stress are termed avoidantly attached (A), whilst infants expressing intense negative emotions and wanting contact, but unable to settle following the separations are classified as resistantly attached (C). Infants who are unable to obtain comfort from the caregiver at times of fearful arousal, and whose behaviour in the reunion episodes appears to lack a clear strategy, may display simultaneous or sequential contradictory behaviours, misdirected, stereotypical movements, extended freezing and direct expression of fear in the presence of the caregiver. These behaviours are rated on a 9-point scale and those with a summary score > 5 are classified as disorganized (D).

Thousands of assessments during the last three decades have allowed the estimation of the frequencies of these infant attachment types across a range of populations. Frequencies in low social risk community samples are typically ~55% secure, ~15% avoidant, ~10% resistant and ~15% disorganized. There is some cross-cultural variation in these frequencies, but differences within any culture can be just as large as between cultures [7]. In high social risk groups exposed to severe deprivation and maltreatment [8] or among infants of adolescent mothers [9] frequencies can be dramatically different with 0-30% secure, 20-50% avoidant and resistant, and as many as 50-80% disorganized infants [8, 10].

Explanations for individual variability in attachment patterns

Environmental factors

Bowlby's attachment theory is a truly environmental theory as it has explained individual differences in attachment patterns (attachment types) by individual variations in caregivers' behaviour. In their seminal study [5], Ainsworth and colleagues found links between observed care-giving behaviour at home and characteristic behaviour patterns in the laboratory-based SSP. They found that the optimal, secure behaviour pattern could be linked to sufficient sensitive responsiveness at home. Sensitivity was conceptually distinguished from responsiveness, with sensitive responses defined as being guided by an appropriate interpretation of infants' signals and changing needs. The avoidant pattern could be related to rejecting, dismissing or neglecting responses to infants' signals, especially to those signals expressing negative emotions, while in the background of the resistant pattern, unreliable, inconsistent care was identified.

Although many studies demonstrated a significant link between early care and attachment, studies varied greatly regarding in estimates of the strength of the relationship. De Wolff and van IJzendoorn [11] reviewed 66 studies to evaluate effect sizes in relation to the methodology used for assessing caregivers' sensitivity. They showed that caregiver sensitivity has been defined and operationalised in many different ways over the preceding thirty years, but however measured, it was far from being an exclusive determinant of the quality of attachment. Indeed, sensitivity accounted for less variance than was expected (effect sizes 0.24-0.32).

Over the last three decades, it has been shown that different demographic risk factors, especially if accumulated may effect the development of attachment, presumably through their proximal or distal influence on parenting [12]. Income and family size, parental age and education, major stressful events, such as loss of a parent, birth of a sibling, severe illness, marital relationships and breakdown affect the quality of attachment relationships [13–19].

It has been expected that secure attachment is promoted by the psychological health of parents, especially mothers. Empirical studies have provided contradictory results, but the majority found that mothers depressed postnatally were more likely to develop insecure attachment relationships with their infants [20–23]. Studies in general have not been able to find direct associations of mother-infant attachment with child care arrangements and with mothers' social support systems [12], but in high social risk groups, lack of support correlated with higher rates of insecure attachment relationships [24–26], while extensive support was found to promote security [27, 28]. Security of mother-infant attachment has been found to be related to mother's mental state with respect to close (attachment) relationships. In several studies, the security of maternal attachment representations, as assessed by the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) [29], has been found to be significantly related to mother-infant attachment security [30].

It has also been shown that while isolated individual risk factors may not have a significant effect on parent-child attachment, the accumulation of adversity may result in sub-optimal relationship development and insecurity of infant attachment [12]. Raikes and Thompson [31] have tested the effect of multiple social and economical factors on attachment and confirmed that their effects were mediated by mothers' care-giving behaviour. In several longitudinal studies, cumulative risk indices and life stress were used to explain discontinuities in attachment security through the life course [32–34].

Friedman and Boyle [35] have recently reviewed attachment-related findings from the longitudinal NICHD Study of Early Child Care (SECC) aiming to identify effects of timing, extent, quality, and type of child care experiences on children's development in a large sample, using well-controlled methodology. The study followed the development of more than one thousand children, of varied backgrounds, from birth and periodically assessed attachment to their mothers. Family SES and maternal sensitivity, especially in responding to the child's distress, predicted the quality of attachment at 15 months [36, 37] and at 36 months of age [38] confirming the previously found moderate strength of the relationship. Recurring symptoms of maternal depression across the first three years predicted higher prevalence of insecure attachment at age 36 months [39]. Analysis of effects of non-maternal care, a special focus of the NICHD study, confirmed the lack of main effects of children's age of entry, quality of care and length of time children spent in non-maternal care, but also revealed interaction effects. The study found higher rates of insecurity if a low quality of non-maternal care was combined with low maternal sensitivity and more time spent in child care. Altogether, the conclusion of the study was that, under some circumstances, early non-maternal child care can be an environmental risk factor for attachment security and compromised later development.

Environmental effects on disorganized attachment

Attachment disorganisation became a focus of developmental research when rarely occurring incoherent and contradictory infant behaviours, not fitting the Ainsworth categories, appeared to be predominant among maltreated or otherwise deprived groups of infants and young children [6, 40]. Infants whose attachment strategy collapses even under the mild stress of brief separation experienced in the Strange Situation and who show high degree of incoherence and disorganisation upon reunion with their caregivers comprise on average 15% of typical populations and as high as 50-80% of high social risk groups [8]. Early disorganised attachment also proved to be one of the rare early predictors of subsequent childhood behaviour problems [41–44] and adolescent psychopathology, such as dissociative symptoms and borderline personality disorder [45, 46]. Regarding the parenting background, Main and Hesse [47] proposed that caregivers of infants displaying disorganized attachment showed bouts of "frightened, threatening, and dissociative" behaviour in interactions with their infants, and this was confirmed by later studies [48–50]. In addition, Lyons-Ruth and colleagues [51] identified a broader spectrum of anomalous parental behaviour contributing to attachment disorganisation, including also communication errors, role confusion and extreme withdrawal. To some extent, these behaviours were also found in low-social-risk groups of mothers [43, 52–55]. Using NICHD SECC data, Campbell and colleagues [39] have shown that chronic depression combined with low maternal sensitivity is associated with a higher prevalence of disorganised attachment in 3-year-old children. Mothers' unresolved trauma or loss has been considered as a potential source of disruptions in maternal care-giving behaviour [43, 47, 50, 56]. According to a recent meta-analysis, however, anomalies of caregiver's mental state and behaviour had only low explanatory power in accounting for attachment disorganization [49]. Including 12 studies examining relations between maternal unresolved loss and trauma, anomalous parenting and disorganised attachment, moderate effect sizes were found for both links between maternal unresolved mental state and anomalous behaviour and infant disorganized attachment (r = .26 and .21, respectively), as well as for the link between mother's anomalous behaviour and infant disorganization (r = .34).

Biological contribution to individual variations in infant attachment

Newborns' biologically based capabilities of self-regulation of arousal and distress states have an immediate impact on parents. The great individual variation in these capabilities can be described by dimensions such as the infant's disposition for distress or negative emotionality, irritability and soothability [57, 58]. There is a long tradition of two different interpretations of the role of infant temperament in the formation of attachment relationships. Attachment theorists have suggested that temperament has no direct effect on the quality of attachment, since infant characteristics such as difficult temperament can be accommodated by sensitive caregivers, who can still foster secure attachment relationships [59]. Temperament researchers, on the other hand, have kept emphasizing that infant-caregiver interactions in the Strange Situation reflect the infant's temperament rather than the quality of the relationship [60]. In their extensive review, Vaughn and Bost [61] argue that temperament and attachment are separate constructs, and studies showing inter-relationships on the one hand, and independence on the other result from different conceptualisations and assessments of both. There is a body of empirical research results, which has demonstrated relations between attachment quality and infant irritability, proneness to distress or stress regulation [26, 58, 62–64]. Based on their review of literature, Mangelsdorf and Frosch [65] have suggested that effects of infant temperament on attachment may be indirect and moderated by other maternal and social variables.

Several studies have found that newborn behavioural measures are related to later secure and insecure attachment classifications [66–70]. These studies are particularly interesting because neonatal measures reflect minimal social experience. Results from the Regensburg longitudinal studies have shown that poor neonatal behavioural organisation was related to disorganised behaviour in the Strange Situation at 18 months of age [71, 72]. This, together with findings that some of the behaviours characteristic of attachment disorganisation have also been found in children with developmental disorders [73, 74] imply that biological factors such as the infant's capability to organise environmental stimuli, communicate and regulate internal states and behaviour may also contribute to the development of disorganised attachment. Summarizing the empirical evidence, Barnett and colleagues [75] suggested a two-dimensional model in which both biological vulnerability as well as adverse environment might contribute to the development of atypical (disorganised) attachment behaviour.

Twin studies of attachment security and disorganisation

Since the study of parent-child attachment was so strongly driven by the environmental theory, a quantitative behaviour-genetic approach has only recently been used to investigate heritable and environmental variance components of attachment security. Most of the existing twin studies of attachment overviewed here have not been designed to focus on attachment, therefore they are quite heterogeneous regarding children's age and the method of assessment. In the Louisville Twin Study (LTS), Finkel and Matheny [76] found a significant difference between concordances of attachment classifications of 99 monozygotic (MZ) and 108 dizygotic (DZ) two-year-old twins. Model-fitting resulted in estimates of 25% genetic and 75% non-shared environmental effects. In another study of 57 MZ and 53 same-sex DZ twins aged 3.5 years, O'Connor and Croft [77] reported results of genetic model-fitting: variance estimates due to genetic, shared and non-shared environmental factors were 14%, 32% and 53%, respectively, but the genetic effect on secure vs. insecure attachment was not significant. Comparing 57 MZ and 81 DZ twin pairs, Bokhorst and colleagues [78] found only unique environmental factors accounting for the variance in disorganised vs. organised attachment, while both shared and non-shared environmental effects accounted for the variance in secure vs. insecure attachment. In a model-fitting analysis using data from 485 twin pairs, Roisman and Fraley [79] have also emphasized the role of environment (parenting quality) in accounting for the variability in toddlers' observed secure-base behaviour. Using staff/parent-rated zygosity, a model containing only shared (C) and non-shared (E) environmental variances "was able to explain the data just as well as the full [ACE] model" (p. 835) providing a heritability estimate of 0.17. Finally, using questionnaire data of attachment disorder behaviours in a very large community sample of 13,472 twins, both twin correlations and model-fitting results suggested a strong genetic influence on attachment disorder behaviour, especially in boys [80].

It is important to recognize the power constraints of quantitative genetic studies such as those using twin comparisons. These analyses aim at assessing heritability; that is, estimating how much of the population variance is due to genetic effects (the rest is environmental variance and measurement error). The twin method is based on comparison of twin correlations or concordances. If genetic effects are important for the trait in question, then correlation or concordance between monozygotic twins should be significantly greater than that between dizygotic twins. Limited sensitivity is inherent in the methodology based on detecting significant differences between twin correlations. This is not such a great limitation for many psychological and psychiatric phenotypes with substantial heritabilities of around 0.5, but may cause problems in detecting smaller, yet potentially important genetic effects. (It is estimated that any specific gene effect accounts for less than 1% of the variance of complex traits [81].) The power limitations of relatively small twin studies are not trivial. Statistical power analysis by Visscher [82] has addressed the questions of rejecting CE models within the classical twin design when the true model is ACE, i.e. there is a significant genetic variance. The simulation results have shown that necessary sample sizes become fairly large at 0.2 heritability except when shared environmental variance exceeds 0.5-0.6. Visscher and colleagues [83] have provided a web-based power calculation tool for determining the minimum number of twin pairs required to detect A and C in an ACE model of given parameters. Unfortunately, all published twin studies of attachment seem to be underpowered for detecting heritability (A), but most are powerful enough for detecting large environmental contributions.

In addition, behaviour-genetic analyses usually employ main effects models dividing up the total phenotypic variance into additive (or dominant) genetic, shared and non-shared environmental components. They normally do not separate gene-environment interactions (genetic sensitivity to environments) and gene-environment correlations (arising from experiences correlated with genetic propensities). Finally, quantitative genetic modelling does not go beyond the extent to which genetic factors influence behavioural traits, that is they are not informative about specific genes or specific environmental factors affecting the behaviours in question.

Molecular genetic studies of attachment

The development of molecular genetic methodology started to shift the focus of study towards locating and identifying specific genes underlying genetic effects evident in twin and adoptive studies, even before sequencing the human genome was completed. The widely used strategy of allele association investigates correlations of gene variants with phenotypes, i.e., it probes if individuals with a trait of interest carry a specific gene variant more frequently than individuals in a control group (case-control studies). The method of allele association has been quickly and productively applied in the area of multi-factorial mental illnesses for which genetic components have long been demonstrated by large heritability estimates. Genes regulating the cerebral levels of important neurotransmitters (dopamine, serotonin, GABA, etc.) or signal transmission efficiency (neurotransmitter receptors and genes) have been targeted in association studies of major psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorders, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and autism [84], as well as of personality traits [85].

The first investigation of the specific genetic background of attachment behaviour showed an association between D4 dopamine receptor (DRD4) gene polymorphism and infants' attachment behaviour [86]. The level of dopamine and the density of dopamine receptors in the PFC increase between 6-12 months of infant life, when many of these functions go through intensive development [87] and when the development of first attachment relationships typically takes place. The highly polymorphic DRD4 gene has a number of frequent functional variants in the populations [88]. One of these is a variable number 48 base pair tandem repeat (48 bp VNTR) in the coding region of the gene [89]. Receptor molecules coded by the 7-repeat allele have been found to have a lower potency for dopamine-mediated coupling to adenylate cyclase than receptors encoded by the other frequent, 2- or 4-repeat forms [90], and more recent results suggest an additional effect of the 48 bp VNTR on gene expression [91, 92]. Allele association studies have linked the 48 bp repeat polymorphism of the DRD4 gene with normal variations of neonatal, infant, and adult temperament [85], but also with clinical hyperactivity (ADHD) [93, 94].

In the longitudinal Budapest Infant-Parent Study (BIPS), a relative over-representation of the 7-repeat variant of the 48 bp VNTR polymorphism was found in the group of infants displaying insecure-disorganised attachment behaviour with their mothers in the Strange Situation [86]. The estimated relative risk for disorganised attachment among children carrying the 7-repeat allele was four-fold, with the frequency of the 7-repeat allele being 67% in disorganised infants as opposed to 20% in securely attached infants [95], and with 50% frequencies in the insecure-avoidant and resistant groups. Subsequently, we also reported an interaction between the structural 48 bp repeat polymorphism and the -521 C/T promoter polymorphism in the same group of infants: the association between disorganised attachment and the 7-repeat allele was enhanced by the presence of the -521T allele [96]. While Dutch researchers have failed to replicate this association [97, 98], parental genetic data and family-based analyses in the Hungarian sample showed a highly significant non-transmission of the 7-repeat allele (and the -521T~7-repeat haplotype) to securely attached infants, as well as a trend for preferential transmission to disorganised infants [95]. The preferential non-transmission of the 7-repeat allele to securely attached infants suggested that not carrying this allele might, in fact, have a protective effect, favouring the development of secure early attachment in this sample.

Gene-environment interaction effects on infant attachment

Specific genetic effects on phenotypes may be conditional on specific environments and thus be undetected in other environments. Similarly, exposure to specific environments are often influenced by choices that depend on the individuals' genetic make-up. Such interplay between genes and environment may channel individual development into different trajectories early in life and affect the long-term development of mental health and disorder. A shift from association studies targeting genetic main effects towards investigating the interaction of genetic and environmental influences is reflected by the accumulating evidence for such processes being reported in recent literature [99]. Important reports are emerging, describing the moderation of behavioural responses to early rearing conditions by specific genotypes. For example, in the Dunedin study, Caspi and colleagues found that links between childhood maltreatment and later psychological maladjustment were moderated by genetic factors [100]. The functional polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene affecting enzyme activity moderated the relation between early maltreatment and later antisocial behaviour in males. In the same population, the 5-HTTLPR regulatory polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene was shown to moderate the effect of early maltreatment on adult depression in both sexes [101]. Both findings have since been replicated [102–104]. Importantly, in these studies, the genetic factors had no main effects on the outcome and the genetic influence was detected only when the environmental measure of maltreatment was included in the analyses.

Our own study of the genetic effects on disorganised attachment has been extended by investigating the interplay between DRD4 gene polymorphism and maternal behaviour [54]. In order to increase the range of environmental and behavioural measures, we have done this in collaboration with the Boston-based US research group led by Dr. Karlen Lyons-Ruth, an expert on disorganised attachment in infants and atypical behaviour of mothers. Demographic risk, DRD4 7-repeat genotype, levels of atypical maternal behaviour and infant disorganised attachment were combined across the middle income, low risk Hungarian and the low income, high social risk US samples (N = 96 and 42, respectively). We found that, in the combined sample, disorganised attachment was related to both cumulative demographic risk and maternal atypical behaviour, but the main effect of infant 7-repeat genotype on disorganised attachment was no longer significant. However, the relation between maternal atypical behaviour and infant disorganisation was moderated by infant DRD4 genotype. The relationship was strong in the group of infants lacking the 7-repeat variant; as expected, mothers showing a low level of anomalous behaviour had infants displaying a low level of disorganisation, and conversely, infants of highly atypical mothers showed a high level of disorganised attachment behaviour. In contrast, the level of disorganisation in infants who carried the 7-repeat allele was at an intermediate level and unrelated to the degree of maternal atypical behaviour. Infants carrying the 7-repeat allele thus seemed to be less sensitive to maternal behaviour. At the same time, the previously reported main effect of the 7-repeat genotype on attachment disorganisation was restricted to the group with mothers low on atypical behaviour. This pattern of results was also present in the separate US and the Hungarian samples. We hypothesised that functional variations in the DRD4 gene expressed preferentially in brain regions of the reward circuit might modulate sensitivity to maternal stimuli, which in turn might result in infants' differential sensitivity to aspects of care-giving behaviour.

Results of this first molecular genetic study of infant attachment [86, 96] seem to have transformed the attachment field by increasing interest in studying genetic and gene-environment interaction effects. A number of research groups have genotyped participants of previous and ongoing attachment studies, or have begun including genetic markers in the design of new studies. A further investigation of the DRD4 48 bp VNTR showed that infant genotype moderated the previously reported intergenerational transmission of mothers' unresolved trauma to disorganised attachment: the link was significant for infants carrying the 7-repeat allele only [105].

Studies have been extended by investigating associations with other candidate genes. The polymorphic serotonin transporter gene, coding for one of the important regulator of the synaptic level of the neurotransmitter serotonin, has been linked previously to anxiety-related traits [106] and affective disorders [107]. The 5-HTTLPR repeat polymorphism in the promoter region of the gene affects gene expression, that is the number of available serotonin transporter molecules. The 'short' 5-HTTLPR allele has been associated with low serotonin metabolism and behavioural problems in infant and juvenile Rhesus monkeys reared with peers only, but not in monkeys who were reared with their mothers and peers during infancy. In contrast, monkeys carrying only the 'long' allele showed normal metabolism and behavioural functioning, regardless of their early rearing history [108]. A similar protective effect of the homozygous 'long/long' (l/l) genotype or else a buffering effect of maternal behaviour was found in human studies of infant attachment. For infants with a 'short' allele, variation in mothers' responsiveness was significantly associated with attachment security [109] or attachment disorganisation [110, 111]. For infants homozygous for the 'long' allele (l/l), there was no association between maternal responsiveness and attachment security or disorganisation.

The gene-environment interaction effects on attachment reported in the above-cited publications are consistent with Belsky's differential susceptibility hypothesis [112], i.e., children's susceptibility to care-giving experience seems to be moderated by genetic factors. Unfortunately, all these attachment studies, as usual, involved relatively small sample sizes. In the light of the latest meta-analyses questioning the 5-HTTLPR main effects [113] and 5-HTTLPR by stressful environment interaction effects [114], these initial investigations of genetic and gene-environment interaction effects on attachment need to be confirmed by larger studies, possibly including multiple polymorphisms of multiple candidate genes.

Genetic moderation of effects of early mother-child relationship on children's problem behaviour

Following up their above mentioned study [109], Kochanska and colleagues [115] found that the 5-HTTLPR promoter genotype moderated not only the relationship between maternal responsiveness and attachment security to the mother, but also the relationship of attachment security with children's later ability for self-regulation at 2, 3 and 4 years of age. Insecurely attached children with a 'short' 5-HTTLPR allele (s/s and s/l) developed poor regulatory capacities, but those who were securely attached did not show a deficit compared to children who were homozygous for the 'long' allele (l/l). For children homozygous for the 'long' 5-HTTLPR allele, attachment security was not related to self-regulation. The authors' interpretation that secure attachment relationship can serve as a protective factor in the presence of risk conferred by a genotype might however be confounded by their previously reported finding [109] that infants with l/l genotype were predominantly securely attached. It seems that in this case, consistent with other primate and human studies, the 5-HTTLPR l/l genotype has a protective effect in the face of parenting risk.

In a small-scale study, (N = 47), infants' DRD4 genotype was found to moderate the relationship between maternal insensitivity and externalising child behaviour: children who carried the 7-repeat allele and had relatively insensitive mothers showed the highest level of externalising behaviour [116]. In a further study, the same researchers investigated the effect of children's DRD4 genotype on the efficacy of parenting intervention in a pre-selected 'externalising' sample [117]. At the follow-up stage, they found that intervention aimed at increasing parental sensitivity and positive discipline was effective in reducing externalisation for children with a 7-repeat allele, but not for children without the 7-repeat allele. This findings points to a genetically moderated differential susceptibility to intervention, which might partially explain difficult-to-treat cases often encountered in clinical work.

Methodological problems in molecular genetic studies

Genetic association studies in the field of psychiatric and psychological genetics have been suffering from inconsistencies in replications of results. There can be many reasons for these difficulties, some of which are the result of methodological flaws, for example genotyping errors. Improved measurement of phenotypes and carefully controlled genotype determination can reduce the danger of false negative findings [118]. Failure of replication, however, may have 'legitimate' underlying causes. Initially, replication problems of case-control studies were often attributed to population stratification (case and control groups originating from genetically distinct populations) and to multiple testing without appropriate corrections, which could result in spurious associations. These problems have been largely dealt with by using genetic data from families [119] and more stringent statistical testing [120]. However, the real problem might be that more complex behavioural phenotypes are affected by many genes, each with only a small effect accounting for only 1% or even less of the phenotypic variance [81], therefore inadequate statistical power is a serious problem. Likewise, different environment interactions across samples can also hinder replication of an association. A considerable genetic heterogeneity is expected within and among studies, as different combinations of the various alleles of the multiple genes and epistatic gene interactions may produce similar phenotypes. This heterogeneity may go undetected as often in smaller studies only a few genetic markers are investigated. According to Greene and colleagues [121], real associations may not replicate in independent samples if they are part of a larger epistatic interaction. In these cases, small sample differences in allele frequency at an interacting locus may impact the power negatively, so the originally reported effect may not replicate or even reversed. The recommendation is to check for interaction with other polymorphisms (see, for example the replicability of 'novelty seeking', where interaction of at least three gene loci made replication uncertain [122].

As mentioned above, some of these problems can be tackled by increasing study sizes and combining study samples, increasing the number of carefully chosen candidate polymorphisms or as Plomin and Davis suggest [81] employing the newly available genome-wide association (GWA) strategy. In GWA studies many hundred thousand single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) across the genome of many (often more than 1000) individuals are genotyped. Comparing profiles of 'case' and 'control' groups, SNPs associated with caseness can be identified. GWA studies of type II diabetes identified nearly 20, robustly replicating gene loci. The associated alleles were common in the studied populations and all had only small effects on disease risk [123]. Although, this seems to be the "future of genetics in psychology and psychiatry" [81], GWA is almost certainly unfeasible in the field of attachment studies.

Animal research pointing to new directions: environmental modification of brain anatomy and gene activity, epigenetic effects of parenting

Studies in animal models have found convincing evidence for the critical impact of early emotional experiences. Studies from the 1950s showed that even short separations of young rodent pups from their mother have profound and persistent effects on behaviour and physiological stress reactivity [124]. In the last two decades, brain development shaped by the interplay of genetic predispositions and experience-induced adaptation has been extensively studied primarily in the context of stress elicited by early separation from the primary caregiver.

An avian model of early parent-offspring bonding is filial imprinting in precocious birds which is accompanied by extensive reorganization in the frontal lobe. Domestic chicks imprinted on artificial stimuli in experimental settings showed increased synaptic connectivity in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), which seems to be important for storage of memory acquired during imprinting [125, 126]. Bock and Braun [127] showed that successful imprinting in chick is accompanied by extensive pruning of excitatory spine synapses in other associative forebrain regions. Imprinted animals later responded to the presentation of the learned stimulus with enhanced brain electrical and metabolic activity (see Sullivan et al. [128] for a review).

This experience-dependent development of neural connections serves the adaptive response of the offspring to its actual normative environment. However, in case of adverse early influences, the same plasticity may lead to altered neural development with long-lasting behavioural and physiological effects. There has been accumulating evidence for the importance of parental care, especially tactile stimulation in the subsequent development of infants of mammalian species. Separation from the mother in rodents induces physiological and behavioural responses including vocalisation and searching behaviour, corticosterone hormone release and inhibition of metabolism related to growth and later stress reactivity [129, 130]. Bock and colleagues [131] found synaptic changes in the prefrontal cortex of rat pups exposed to 1-hour-long separations from their mother. These changes were region-specific, depended on timing of the separations during the first weeks of the pups' life, and they also found a possible link between synaptic changes and endocrine function. A further study on the semi-precocious rodent Octodon degus [132] showed that short, repeated separations in the first three weeks of life resulted in significant alterations of density of neurons releasing corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) in brain regions involved in emotion regulation. More sustained early social isolation of young degus altered the serotonergic and dopaminergic cortical innervation in the orbital prefrontal cortex possibly reflecting different functioning of these monoamine transmitter systems in result of parental deprivation [133].

These few examples selected from an extensive research field illustrates the critical effects of early stress on brain development. The mechanisms underlying alterations in brain development are far from being understood in animals and even less in humans, but new molecular techniques and imaging methods may hold the key to growing knowledge of normal and corrupted developmental trajectories.

In relation to the association of infant attachment with a promoter polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene, we have mentioned that gene expression may be affected by variation in the DNA sequence of the regulatory region of the gene. There is, however, increasing evidence for changes in gene expression through DNA and chromatin modifications without a change in the inherited nucleotide sequence. Epigenetic modifications of promoter regions that influence the transcription of genes coding for crucial protein products can be induced by the environment. Although the mechanisms through which these modifications occur have yet to be fully explored, one important process is environmentally induced DNA methylation leading to transcriptional silencing. It has also been shown that these environmentally mediated effects on gene expression tend to persist in the individual's lifetime and can even be transmitted from one generation to the next [134]. The pioneering research by Meaney and colleagues [135] has shown that, in rats, stable individual differences in maternal care are related to individual differences in pups' later behaviour and stress reactivity. Cross-fostering experiments have proved that these maternal effects are not heritable in the narrow sense, but are consequences of the characteristics of maternal care received in the first week of life. Meaney and colleagues have found that variation in early care affects the expression of the glucocorticoid receptor gene in the hippocampus by differential methylation of the promoter region of the gene leading to stable differences in offspring stress reactivity persisting through adulthood [136]. Moreover, they have shown that, through differential patterns of DNA methylation of the promoter region and thereby differential expression of the estrogen receptor alpha gene in the medial preoptic area (MPOA) of the brain, this individual variation of early experience influences adult reproductive behaviour, suggesting a mechanism for intergenerational transmission of the pattern of maternal care [137, 138]. Results of animal work cannot be translated directly to humans, but initial findings of recent research suggest an effect of parental care on epigenetic regulation in the human brain. The methylation pattern of the promoter of glucocorticoid receptor gene in the hippocampus of suicide victims with a history of childhood abuse has been found to differ from that of suicide victims with no childhood abuse or control subjects [139]. Thus, there is a good reason for hypothesising that epigenetic modification of gene expression plays a role in the development of early mother-infant relationship, even if at present it seems impossible to study these processes in human infants.

Conclusion

There is a considerable individual variation in infants' attachment behaviour with their primary caregivers. Attachment theory, as first conceived, gave an environmental explanation for this variation, regarding caregivers' sensitive responsiveness to infant signals as the crucial factor for the development of optimal, secure attachment. This exclusively environmental explanation has recently been challenged by studies that have included physiological and genetic measures. Although twin studies of attachment are providing mixed results, molecular genetic studies are showing that specific genetic polymorphisms of the dopaminergic and serotonergic neurotransmisson systems may moderate the statistically identified links between caregivers' behaviour and infant attachment. These gene-environment interaction effects, pointing to genetically-based differential susceptibilities to care-giving environments, have been more often found for disorganised attachment, which is an extreme form of insecure attachment predictive of subsequent behavioural problems and psychopathology. Because the results of the as-yet few and relatively small-scale studies are not wholly consistent, there is a clear need for larger-scale and carefully designed studies that examine multiple polymorphisms of multiple candidate genes. More recent research on the epigenetic modification of gene expression by early maternal care in animals suggests the possibility of similar processes affecting human development. The investigation of such processes in humans, although not feasible at present, would offer an opportunity to gain a deeper understanding of developmental psychopathology and the intergenerational transmission of attachment and parenting.

References

  1. Bowlby J: Attachment and Loss: Attachment. 1969, New York: Basic Books, 1.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Brazelton TB, Nugent JK: Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. 1995, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 3

    Google Scholar 

  3. Ainsworth MDS: Attachments across the life span. Bull N Y Acad Med. 1985, 61: 792-812.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Ainsworth M, Wittig B: Attachment and exploratory behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Determinants of infant behavior. Edited by: Foss BM. 1969, London: Methuen, 113-136.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Ainsworth MD, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S: Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. 1978, Hillsdale: Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  6. Main M, Solomon J: Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. Edited by: Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM. 1990, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 121-160.

    Google Scholar 

  7. van IJzendoorn MH, Sagi A: Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and contextual dimensions. Handbook of attachment. Theory, research, and clinical applications. Edited by: Cassidy J, Shaver PR. 1999, New York: The Guilford Press, 713-734.

    Google Scholar 

  8. van IJzendoorn MH, Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ: Disorganized attachment in early childhood: meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Dev Psychopathol. 1999, 11: 225-249. 10.1017/S0954579499002035.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Madigan S, Moran G, Pederson DR: Unresolved states of mind, disorganized attachment relationships, and disrupted interactions of adolescent mothers and their infants. Dev Psychol. 2006, 42: 293-304. 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. van IJzendoorn MH, Goldberg S, Kroonenberg PM, Frenkel OJ: The relative effects of maternal and child problems on the quality of attachment: A meta-analysis of attachment in clinical samples. Child Dev. 1992, 63: 840-858. 10.2307/1131237.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. DeWolff M, van IJzendoorn MH: Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Dev. 1997, 68: 571-591. 10.2307/1132107.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Belsky J: Interactional and contextual determinants of attachment security. Handbook of attachment. Theory, research, and clinical applications. Edited by: Cassidy J, Shaver PR. 1999, New York: The Guilford Press, 249-286.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Cummings EM, Davies PT: Effects of marital conflict on children: Recent advances and emerging themes in process-oriented research. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002, 43: 31-63. 10.1111/1469-7610.00003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Gloger-Tippelt GS, Huerkamp M: Relationship change at the transition to parenthood and the security of infant-mother attachment. Int J Behav Dev. 1998, 22: 633-655. 10.1080/016502598384306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Moss E, Cyr C, Dubois-Comtois K: Attachment at early school age and developmental risk: examining family contexts and behavior problems of controlling-caregiving, controlling-punitive, and behaviorally disorganized children. Dev Psychol. 2004, 40: 519-532. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.519.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Nair H, Murray AD: Predictors of attachment security in preschool children from intact and divorced families. J Genet Psychol. 2005, 166: 245-263. 10.3200/GNTP.166.3.245-263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Owen M, Cox M: Marital conflict and the development of infant-parent attachment relationships. J Fam Psychol. 1997, 11: 152-164. 10.1037/0893-3200.11.2.152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Teti DM, Sakin JW, Kucera E, Corns KM, Eiden RD: And baby makes four: Predictors of attachment security among preschool-age firstborns during the transition to siblinghood. Child Dev. 1996, 67: 579-596. 10.2307/1131833.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Zeanah CH, Danis B, Hirschberg L, Benoit D, Miller D, Heller SS: Disorganized attachment associated with partner violence: A research note. Infant Mental Health J. 1999, 20: 77-86. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199921)20:1<77::AID-IMHJ6>3.0.CO;2-S.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. DeMulder EK, Radke-Yarrow M: Attachment with affectively ill and well mothers: Concurrent behavioral correlates. Dev Psychopathol. 1991, 3: 227-242. 10.1017/S0954579400005277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Martins C, Gaffan EA: Effects of early maternal depression on patterns of infant-mother attachment: a meta-analytic investigation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000, 41: 737-746. 10.1111/1469-7610.00661.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Murray L: The impact of postnatal depression on infant development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992, 33: 543-561. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00890.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Teti D, Gelfand D, Messinger D, Isabella R: Maternal depression and the quality of early attachment. Dev Psychol. 1995, 31: 364-376. 10.1037/0012-1649.31.3.364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Crittenden PM: Social network, quality of child rearing and child development. Child Dev. 1985, 56: 1299-1313. 10.2307/1130245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Crnic KA, Greenberg MT, Ragozin AS, Robinson NM, Basham RB: Effects of stress and social support on mothers and premature and full-term infants. Child Dev. 1983, 54: 209-217. 10.2307/1129878.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Crockenberg SB: Infant irritability, mother responsiveness, and social support influences on the security of infant-mother attachment. Child Dev. 1981, 52: 857-869. 10.2307/1129087.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Crockenberg SB: Social support and parenting. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1988, 4: 141-174.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Isabella RA: Origins of maternal role satisfaction and its influences upon maternal interactive behavior and infant-mother attachment. Infant Behav Dev. 1994, 17: 381-388. 10.1016/0163-6383(94)90030-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Main M, Kaplan N, Cassidy J: Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Growing Points of Attachment Theory and Research. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. Edited by: Bretherton I, Waters E. 1985, 50: 66-104. 10.2307/3333827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. van IJzendoorn MH: Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychol Bull. 1995, 117: 387-403. 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.387.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Raikes HA, Thompson RA: Links between contextual risk and attachment: Models of influence. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2005, 26: 440-455. 10.1016/j.appdev.2005.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Bar-Haim Y, Sutton DB, Fox NA, Marvin RS: Stability and change of attachment at 14, 24, and 58 months of age: Behavior, representation, and life events. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000, 41: 381-388. 10.1111/1469-7610.00622.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Hamilton CE: Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from infancy through adolescence. Child Dev. 2000, 71: 690-694. 10.1111/1467-8624.00177.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Waters E, Merrick S, Treboux D, Crowell J, Albersheim L: Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2000, 71: 684-689. 10.1111/1467-8624.00176.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Friedman SL, Boyle DE: Attachment in US children experiencing nonmaternal care in the early 1990s. Attach Hum Dev. 2008, 10: 225-61. 10.1080/14616730802113570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network: The effects of infant child care on infant-mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Dev. 1997, 68: 860-879. [http://www.jstor.org/pss/1132038]

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. McElwain NL, Booth-LaForce C: Maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress as predictors of infant-mother attachment security. J Fam Psychol. 2006, 20: 247-255. 10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network: Child-care and family predictors of preschool attachment and stability from infancy. Dev Psychol. 2001, 37: 847-862. 10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.847.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Campbell SB, Brownell CA, Hungerford A, Spieker SI, Mohan R, Blessing JS: The course of maternal depressive symptoms and maternal sensitivity as predictors of attachment security at 36 months. Dev Psychopathol. 2004, 16: 231-252. 10.1017/S0954579404044499.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lyons-Ruth K, Connell DB, Grunebaum HU, Botein S: Infants at social risk: maternal depression and family support services as mediators of infant development and security of attachment. Child Dev. 1990, 61: 85-98. 10.2307/1131049.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Lyons-Ruth K, Alpern L, Repacholi B: Disorganized infant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial problems as predictors of hostile-aggressive behavior in the preschool classroom. Child Dev. 1993, 64: 572-585. 10.2307/1131270.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Lyons-Ruth K: Attachment relationships among children with aggressive behavior problems: The role of disorganized early attachment patterns. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1996, 64: 64-73. 10.1037/0022-006X.64.1.64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Madigan S, Moran G, Schuengel C, Pederson DR, Otten R: Unresolved maternal attachment representations, disrupted maternal behavior and disorganized attachment in infancy: links to toddler behavior problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007, 48: 1042-1050. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01805.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Zeanah CH, Keyes A, Settles L: Attachment relationship experiences and childhood psychopathology. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003, 1008: 22-30. 10.1196/annals.1301.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Carlson EA: A prospective longitudinal study of attachment disorganization/disorientation. Child Dev. 1998, 69: 1107-1128. [http://cat.inist.fr/?aModele=afficheN&cpsidt=2392248]

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Liotti G: A model of dissociation based on attachment theory and research. J Trauma Dissociation. 2006, 7: 55-73. 10.1300/J229v07n04_04.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Main M, Hesse E: Parent's unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized/disoriented attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism?. Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. Edited by: Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM. 1990, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 161-182.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Abrams KY, Rifkin A, Hesse E: Examining the role of parental frightened/frightening subtypes in predicting disorganized attachment within a brief observational procedure. Dev Psychopathol. 2006, 18: 345-361. 10.1017/S0954579406060184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Madigan S, Bakermans-Kranenburg M, van IJzendoorn M, Moran G, Pederson D, Benoit D: Unresolved states of mind, anomalous parental behavior, and disorganized attachment: a review and meta-analysis of a transmission gap. Attach Hum Dev. 2006, 8: 89-111. 10.1080/14616730600774458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH: Frightening maternal behavior linking unresolved loss and disorganized infant attachment. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1999, 67: 54-63. 10.1037/0022-006X.67.1.54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Lyons-Ruth K, Bronfman E, Parsons E: Maternal frightened, frightening, or atypical behavior and disorganized infant attachment patterns. Atypical attachment in infancy and early childhood among children at developmental risk. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. Edited by: Vondra JI, Barnett D. 1999, 64: 67-96. [http://www.jstor.org/pss/3181559]

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Goldberg S, Benoit D, Blokland K, Madigan S: Atypical maternal behavior, maternal representations, and infant disorganized attachment. Dev Psychopathol. 2003, 15: 239-257. 10.1017/S0954579403000130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Grienenberger JF, Kelly K, Slade A: Maternal reflective functioning, mother-infant affective communication, and infant attachment: exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attach Hum Dev. 2005, 7: 299-311. 10.1080/14616730500245963.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Gervai J, Novak A, Lakatos K, Toth I, Danis I, Ronai Z, Nemoda Z, Sasvari-Szekely M, Bureau JF, Bronfman E, Lyons-Ruth K: Infant genotype may moderate sensitivity to maternal affective communications: Attachment disorganization, quality of care, and the DRD4 gene polymorphism. Soc Neurosci. 2007, 2: 307-319. 10.1080/17470910701391893.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Hesse E, Main M: Frightened, threatening, and dissociative parental behavior in low-risk samples: description, discussion, and interpretations. Dev Psychopathol. 2006, 18: 309-343. 10.1017/S0954579406060172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Jacobvitz D, Leon K, Hazen N: Does expectant mothers' unresolved trauma predict frightened/frightening maternal behavior? Risk and protective factors. Dev Psychopathol. 2006, 18: 363-379. 10.1017/S0954579406060196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Rothbart MK: Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Dev. 1981, 52: 569-578. 10.2307/1129176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. van den Boom D: The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration. Child Dev. 1994, 65: 1457-1477. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00829.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Sroufe LA: Attachment classification from the perspective of infant-caregiver relationships and infant temperament. Child Dev. 1985, 56: 1-14. [http://www.jstor.org/pss/1130168]

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Kagan J: The nature of the child. 1984, New York: Basic Books

    Google Scholar 

  61. Vaughn BE, Bost KK: Attachment and Temperament. Handbook of attachment. Theory, research, and clinical applications. Edited by: Cassidy J, Shaver PR. 1999, New York: The Guilford Press, 198-225.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Gunnar M: Attachment and stress in early development. Attachment and bonding: a new synthesis. Edited by: Carter CS, Ahnert L, Grossmann KE, et al. 2005, Cambridge: The MIT Press, 245-255.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Spangler G, Grossmann KE: Biobehavioral organization in securely and insecurely attached infants. Child Dev. 1993, 64: 1439-1450. 10.2307/1131544.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Spangler G, Schieche M: Emotional and adrenocortical responses of infants to the strange situation: The differential function of emotional expression. Int J Behav Dev. 1998, 22: 681-706. 10.1080/016502598384126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Mangelsdorf SC, Frosch CA: Temperament and attachment: one construct or two?. Adv Child Dev Behav. 1999, 27: 181-220.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Waters E, Vaughn BE, Egeland BR: Individual differences in infant-mother attachment relationships at age one: Antecedents in neonatal behavior in an urban economically disadvantaged sample. Child Dev. 1980, 51: 208-216. 10.2307/1129608.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Egeland B, Farber EA: Infant-mother attachment: Factors related to its development and changes over time. Child Dev. 1984, 55: 753-771. 10.2307/1130127.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Miyake K, Chen S, Campos JJ: Infant temperament, mother's mode of interaction, and attachment in Japan: An interim report. Growing points of attachment theory and research. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. Edited by: Bretherton I, Waters E. 1985, 50: 276-297. 10.2307/3333838.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  69. van den Boom DC: Neonatal irritability and the development of attachment. Temperament in childhood. Edited by: Kohnstamm GA, Bates JE, Rothbart MK. 1989, New York: Wiley, 299-318.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Calkins SD, Fox NA: The relations among infant temperament, security of attachment, and behavioral inhibition at twenty-four months. Child Dev. 1992, 63: 1456-1472. 10.2307/1131568.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Spangler G, Fremmer-Bombik E, Grossmann KE: Social and individual determinants of infant attachment security and disorganization. Infant Ment Health J. 1996, 17: 127-139. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199622)17:2<127::AID-IMHJ3>3.0.CO;2-N.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Spangler G, Grossmann K: Individual and psychological correlates of attachment disorganization in infancy. Attachment disorganization. Edited by: Solomon J, George C. 1999, New York: The Guilford Press, 95-124.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Capps L, Sigman M, Mundy P: Attachment security in children with autism. Dev Psychopathol. 1994, 6: 249-261. 10.1017/S0954579400004569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Vaughn BE, Goldberg S, Atkinson L, Marcovitch S: Quality of toddler-mother attachment in children with Down syndrome: Limits to interpretation of strange situation behavior. Child Dev. 1994, 65: 95-108. 10.2307/1131368.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Barnett D, Butler CM, Vondra JI: Atypical patterns of early attachment: Discussion and future directions. Atypical attachment in infancy and early childhood among children at developmental risk. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. Edited by: Vondra JI, Barnett D. 1999, 64: 172-192. [http://www.jstor.org/pss/3181563]

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Finkel D, Matheny AP: Genetic and environmental influences on a measure of infant attachment security. Twin Res. 2000, 3: 242-250. 10.1375/136905200320565210.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. O'Connor TG, Croft CM: A twin study of attachment in preschool children. Child Dev. 2001, 72: 1501-1511. 10.1111/1467-8624.00362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Bokhorst CL, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Fearon RMP, van IJzendoorn MH, Fonagy P, Schuengel C: The importance of shared environment in mother-infant attachment security: a behavioral genetic study. Child Dev. 2003, 74: 1769-1782. 10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00637.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Roisman GI, Fraley RC: Behavior-genetic study of parenting quality, infant-attachment security, and their covariation in a nationally representative sample. Dev Psychol. 2008, 44: 831-839. 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.831.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Minnis H, Reekie J, Young D, O'Connor T, Ronald A, Gray A, Plomin R: Genetic, environmental and gender influences on attachment disorder behaviours. Br J Psychiatry. 2007, 190: 490-495. 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.019745.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Plomin R, Davis OS: The future of genetics in psychology and psychiatry: microarrays, genome-wide association, and non-coding RNA. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009, 50: 63-71. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01978.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Visscher PM: Power of the classical twin design revisited. Twin Res. 2004, 7: 505-512. 10.1375/1369052042335250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Visscher PM, Gordon S, Neale MC: Power of the classical twin design revisited: II detection of common environmental variance. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2008, 11: 48-54. 10.1375/twin.11.1.48.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Rutter M: Genes and behavior: Nature-nurture interplay explained. 2006, Oxford: Blackwell

    Google Scholar 

  85. Ebstein RP: The molecular genetic architecture of human personality: beyond self-report questionnaires. Mol Psychiatry. 2006, 11: 427-445. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001814.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Lakatos K, Toth I, Nemoda Z, Ney K, Sasvari-Szekely M, Gervai J: Dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) polymorphism is associated with attachment disorganization in infants. Mol Psychiatry. 2000, 5: 633-637. 10.1038/sj.mp.4000773.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Diamond A: A model system for studying the role of dopamine in the prefrontal cortex during early development in humans: early and continuously treated phenylketonuria. Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience. Edited by: Nelson CA, Luciana M. 2001, Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press, 433-472.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Oak JN, Oldenhof J, Van Tol HHM: The dopamine D4 receptor: one decade of research. Eur J Pharmacol. 2000, 405: 303-327. 10.1016/S0014-2999(00)00562-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Van Tol HH, Wu CM, Guan HC, Ohara K, Bunzow JR, Civelli O, Kennedy J, Seeman P, Niznik HB, Jovanovic V: Multiple dopamine D4 receptor variants in the human population. Nature. 1992, 358: 149-152. 10.1038/358149a0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Asghari V, Sanyal S, Buchwalt S, Paterson A, Jovanovic V, Van Tol HHM: Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. J Neurochem. 1995, 65: 1157-1165.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Schoots O, Van Tol HHM: The human dopamine D4 receptor repeat sequences modulate expression. Pharmacogenomics J. 2003, 3: 343-348. 10.1038/sj.tpj.6500208.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. van Craenenbroeck K, Clark SD, Cox MJ, Oak JN, Liu F, Van Tol HHM: Folding efficiency is rate-limiting in dopamine D4 receptor biogenesis. J Biol Chem. 2005, 280: 19350-19357. 10.1074/jbc.M414043200.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Faraone SV, Khan SA: Candidate gene studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006, 67 (Suppl 8): 13-20.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Li D, Sham PC, Owen MJ, He L: Meta-analysis shows significant association between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Hum Mol Genet. 2006, 15: 2276-2284. 10.1093/hmg/ddl152.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Gervai J, Nemoda Z, Lakatos K, Ronai Z, Toth I, Ney K, Sasvari-Szekely M: Transmission Disequilibrium Tests confirm the link between DRD4 gene polymorphism and infant attachment. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005, 132B: 126-130. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Lakatos K, Nemoda Z, Toth I, Ronai Z, Ney K, Sasvari Szekely M, Gervai J: Further evidence for the role of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) in attachment disorganization: interaction of the III exon 48 bp repeat and the -521 C/T promoter polymorphisms. Mol Psychiatry. 2002, 7: 27-31. 10.1038/sj/mp/4000986.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH: No association of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and -521 C/T promoter polymorphisms with infant attachment disorganization. Attach Hum Dev. 2004, 6: 211-218. 10.1080/14616730412331281584.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH: Research Review: genetic vulnerability or differential susceptibility in child development: the case of attachment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007, 48: 1160-1173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Rutter M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A: Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: multiple varieties but real effects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006, 47: 226-261. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01557.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW, Taylor A, Poulton R: Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science. 2002, 297: 851-854. 10.1126/science.1072290.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, McClay J, Mill J, Martin J, Braithwaite A, Poulton R: Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003, 301: 386-389. 10.1126/science.1083968.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Foley DL, Eaves LJ, Wormley B, Silberg JL, Maes HH, Kuhn J, Riley B: Childhood adversity, monoamine oxidase A genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004, 61: 738-744. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.7.738.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Kaufman J, Yang BZ, Douglas-Palumberi H, Houshyar S, Lipschitz D, Krystal JH, Gelernter J: Social supports and serotonin transporter gene moderate depression in maltreated children. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004, 101: 17316-17321. 10.1073/pnas.0404376101.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Taylor A, Williams B, Newcombe R, Craig IW, Moffitt TE: MAOA, maltreatment, and gene-environment interaction predicting children's mental health: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2006, 11: 903-913. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001851.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ: DRD4 7-repeat polymorphism moderates the association between maternal unresolved loss or trauma and infant disorganization. Attach Hum Dev. 2006, 8: 291-307. 10.1080/14616730601048159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Sen S, Burmeister M, Ghosh D: Meta-analysis of the association between a serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety-related personality traits. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004, 127B: 85-89. 10.1002/ajmg.b.20158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Lotrich FE, Pollock BG: Meta-analysis of serotonin transporter polymorphisms and affective disorders. Psychiatr Genet. 2004, 14: 121-129. 10.1097/00041444-200409000-00001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Suomi SJ: Risk, resilience, and gene × environment interactions in rhesus monkeys. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006, 1094: 52-62. 10.1196/annals.1376.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Barry RA, Kochanska G, Philibert RA: G × E interaction in the organization of attachment: mothers' responsiveness as a moderator of children's genotypes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008, 49: 1313-1320. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01935.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Spangler G, Zimmermann P: Genetic contribution to attachment disorganization and temperament. 2007 SRCD Biennial Meeting, Boston, Mass, USA. 2007

    Google Scholar 

  111. Spangler G, Johann M, Ronai Z, Zimmermann P: Genetic and environmental influence on attachment disorganization. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009, 50: 952-961. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02054.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Belsky J: Theory testing, effect-size evaluation, and differential susceptibility to rearing influence the case of mothering and attachment. Child Dev. 1997, 68: 598-600. 10.2307/1132110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Munafò MR, Freimer NB, Ng W, Ophoff R, Veijola J, Miettunen J, Järvelin MR, Taanila A, Flint J: 5-HTTLPR genotype and anxiety-related personality traits: a meta-analysis and new data. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009, 150B: 271-281. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30808.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Munafò MR, Durrant C, Lewis G, Flint J: Gene × environment interactions at the serotonin transporter locus. Biol Psychiatry. 2009, 65: 211-219. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.06.009.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  115. Kochanska G, Philibert RA, Barry RA: Interplay of genes and early mother-child relationship in the development of self-regulation from toddler to preschool age. J Child Psychol Psychiatry in press.

  116. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH: Gene-environment interaction of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and observed maternal insensitivity predicting externalizing behavior in preschoolers. Dev Psychobiol. 2006, 48: 406-409. 10.1002/dev.20152.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Pijlman FT, Mesman J, Juffer F: Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers' externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Dev Psychol. 2008, 44: 293-300. 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Cardon LR: Practical barriers to identifying complex trait loci. Behavioral Genetics in the Postgenomic Era. Edited by: Plomin R, DeFries JC, Craig IC, McGuffin P. 2003, Washington, DC: APA Books, 55-69.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  119. Laird NM, Lange C: Family-based methods for linkage and association analysis. Adv Genet. 2008, 60: 219-252. full_text.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Schulze TG, McMahon FJ: Genetic association mapping at the crossroads: Which test and why? Overview and practical guidelines. Am J Med Genet. 2002, 114 (1): 1-11. 10.1002/ajmg.10042.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Greene CS, Penrod NM, Williams SM, Moore JH: Failure to replicate a genetic association may provide important clues about genetic architecture. PLoS ONE. 2009, 4: e5639-10.1371/journal.pone.0005639.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  122. Benjamin J, Osher Y, Kotler M, Gritsenko I, Nemanov L, Belmaker RH, Ebstein RP: Association between tridimensional personality questionnaire (TPQ) traits and three functional polymorphisms: dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4), serotonin transporter promoter region (5-HTTLPR) and catechol O-methyltransferase (COMT). Mol Psychiatry. 2000, 5: 96-100. 10.1038/sj.mp.4000640.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. McCarthy MI, Zeggini E: Genome-wide association studies in type 2 diabetes. Curr Diab Rep. 2009, 9: 164-171. 10.1007/s11892-009-0027-4.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  124. Sánchez MM, Ladd CO, Plotsky PM: Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: evidence from rodent and primate models. Dev Psychopathol. 2001, 13: 419-449. 10.1017/S0954579401003029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  125. Horn G, Bradley P, McCabe BJ: Changes in the structure of synapses associated with learning. J Neurosci. 1985, 5: 3161-3168.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Horn G, Nicol AU, Brown MW: Tracking memory's trace. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001, 98: 5282-5287. 10.1073/pnas.091094798.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Bock J, Braun K: Differential emotional experience leads to pruning of dendritic spines in the forebrain of domestic chicks. Neural Plast. 1998, 6: 17-27. 10.1155/NP.1998.17.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  128. Sullivan R, Wilson DA, Feldon J, Yee BK, Meyer U, Richter-Levin G, Avi A, Michael T, Gruss M, Bock J, Helmeke C, Braun K: The International Society for Developmental Psychobiology annual meeting symposium: Impact of early life experiences on brain and behavioral development. Dev Psychobiol. 2006, 48: 583-602. 10.1002/dev.20170.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  129. Hofer MA: Hidden regulators. Implications for a new understanding of attachment, separation, and loss. Attachment Theory: Social, Developmental and Clinical Perspectives. Edited by: Goldberg S, et al. 1995, Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press, 203-230.

    Google Scholar 

  130. Kuhn CM, Schanberg SM: Responses to maternal separation mechanisms and mediators. Int J Dev Neurosci. 1998, 16: 261-270. 10.1016/S0736-5748(98)00034-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  131. Bock J, Gruss M, Becker S, Braun K: Experience-induced changes of dendritic spine densities in the prefrontal and sensory cortex: correlation with developmental time windows. Cereb Cortex. 2005, 15: 802-808. 10.1093/cercor/bhh181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  132. Becker K, Abraham A, Kindler J, Helmeke C, Braun K: Exposure to neonatal separation stress alters exploratory behavior and corticotropin releasing factor expression in neurons in the amygdala and hippocampus. Dev Neurobiol. 2007, 67: 617-629. 10.1002/dneu.20372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  133. Poeggel G, Nowicki L, Braun K: Early social deprivation alters monoaminergic afferents in the orbital prefrontal cortex of Octodon degus. Neuroscience. 2003, 116: 617-620. 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00751-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  134. Rakyan VK, Beck S: Epigenetic variation and inheritance in mammals. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2006, 16: 573-577. 10.1016/j.gde.2006.09.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  135. Meaney MJ: Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Ann Rev Neurosci. 2001, 24: 1161-1192. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.1161.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  136. Weaver IC, Cervoni N, Champagne FA, D'Alessio AC, Sharma S, Seckl JR, Dymov S, Szyf M, Meaney MJ: Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2004, 7: 847-854. 10.1038/nn1276.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  137. Cameron NM, Champagne FA, Parent C, Fish EW, Ozaki-Kuroda K, Meaney MJ: The programming of individual differences in defensive responses and reproductive strategies in the rat through variations in maternal care. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005, 29: 843-865. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  138. Champagne FA: Epigenetic mechanisms and the transgenerational effects of maternal care. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2008, 29: 386-397. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.03.003.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  139. McGowan PO, Sasaki A, D'Alessio AC, Dymov S, Labonté B, Szyf M, Turecki G, Meaney MJ: Epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor in human brain associates with childhood abuse. Nat Neurosci. 2009, 12: 342-348. 10.1038/nn.2270.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author wishes to thank Krisztina Lakatos, Ildiko Toth and John M. Oates for their comments on the manuscript. This work has been supported by a grant (NK 73551) to the author from the Hungarian Science Fund (OTKA). This paper is based on an invited lecture at the conference on "Early intervention: Bridging the gap between practice and academia" in Stuttgart, Germany, November 27th and 28th 2008.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Judit Gervai.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author declares that they have no competing interests.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gervai, J. Environmental and genetic influences on early attachment. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 3, 25 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1186/1753-2000-3-25

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1753-2000-3-25

Keywords